Interaction between the variety of negative stimuli and the type of go/no-go procedure

  1. Eduardo Polín 1
  2. Vicente Pérez 1
  1. 1 Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia
    info

    Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia

    Madrid, España

    ROR https://ror.org/02msb5n36

Revista:
Anales de psicología

ISSN: 0212-9728 1695-2294

Ano de publicación: 2018

Volume: 34

Número: 2

Páxinas: 378-384

Tipo: Artigo

DOI: 10.6018/ANALESPS.34.2.307401 DIALNET GOOGLE SCHOLAR lock_openDIGITUM editor

Outras publicacións en: Anales de psicología

Obxectivos de Desenvolvemento Sustentable

Resumo

Two experiments were carried out in order to compare the speed of acquisition of visual simple discriminations by pigeons depending on whether the negative stimuli could vary trial by trial (varied condition) or it was always the same stimulus (constant condition). In the first experiment, the subjects were randomly assigned to the conditions and then exposed to a go/no-go procedure in which ―no-go‖ trials were also reinforced. Subsequently, the subjects changed from one condition to the other and were exposed to a new discrimination. In the second experiment, two discriminations were arranged in the same way as in the previous experiment, but, this time, ―no-go‖ trials were not reinforced. In Experiment 1, the acquisition was slower in the varied condition. In Experiment 2, this effect was only observed when the subjects were exposed to the varied condition in the second discrimination. These results suggest, on one hand, that the variety of negative stimuli is an important variable to be considered. And, on the other hand, that the resulting effect on acquisition might be strongly sensitive to the kind of procedure that is used.

Referencias bibliográficas

  • Bowman L. G., Piazza C. C., Fisher W. W., Hagopian L. P., & Kogan J. S. (1997). Assessment of preference for varied versus single reinforcers. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 451–458. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1997.30-451
  • De Rose, J. C., McIlvane, W. J., Dube, W. V., Galpin, V. C., & Stoddard, L. T. (1988). Emergent simple discrimination established by indirect relation to differential consequences. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 1-20. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1988.50-1
  • Dube, W. V., McIlvane, W. J., Maguire, R. W., Mackay, H. A., y Stoddard, L. T. (1989). Stimulus class formation and stimulus—reinforcer relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 65-76. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1989.51-65
  • Egel, A. L. (1980). The effects of constant vs. varied reinforcer presentation on responding by autistic children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 30, 455-463. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(80)90050-8
  • Egel, A. L. (1981). Reinforcer variation: Implications for motivating developmentally disabled children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 14, 345-350. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1981.14-345
  • Farthing, G. W. (1974). Behavioral contrast with multiple positive and negative stimuli on a continuum. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 22, 419-425. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1974.22-419
  • Mandler, J. M. (1970). Two-choice discrimination learning using multiple stimuli. Learning and Motivation, 1, 261-266. doi: 10.1016/0023-9690(70)90150-5
  • Mandler, J. M. (1973). Multiple stimulus discrimination learning. III. What is learned? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25, 112-123. doi: 10.1080/14640747308400329
  • Milo, J. S., Mace, F. C., & Nevin, J. A. (2010). The effects of constant versus varied reinforcers on preference and resistance to change. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 93, 385-394. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2010.93-385
  • Mullins, G. P., & Winefield, A. H. (1979). The relative importance of responses to S+ and S- in simultaneous discrimination learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 31, 329-338. doi: 10.1080/14640747908400731
  • Pérez, V., & Polín, E. (2016). Simple discrimination training and conditional discrimination response. Anales de Psicología, 32, 250-255. doi: 10.6018/analesps.32.1.189471
  • Polín, E., & Pérez, V. (2017). The effect of varied reinforcement on acquisition and extinction speed. Psicothema, 29, 83-90. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2016.153
  • Saunders, R. R., & Green, G. (1999). A discrimination analysis of training-structure effects on stimulus equivalence outcomes. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 72, 117-137. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1999.72-117
  • Schaeffer, B. H., & Shandro, N. (1969). Discrimination acquisition and reversal in the rat as a function of number of negative stimuli. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 69, 201. doi: 10.1037/h0027952
  • Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton.
  • Skinner, B. F. (1948). ‘Superstition’ in the pigeon. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 168. doi: 10.1037/h0055873.
  • Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan.
  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
  • Skinner, B. F. (1969). Contingencies of reinforcement. New York: Appleton.
  • Steinman W. M. (1968a) Response rate and varied reinforcement: Reinforcers of similar strengths. Psychonomic Science, 10, 35. doi: 10.3758/BF03331393
  • Steinman W. M. (1968b) Response rate and varied reinforcement: Reinforcers of different strengths. Psychonomic Science, 10, 36. doi: 10.3758/BF03331394
  • Thrailkill, E. A., Epstein, L. H., & Bouton, M. E. (2015). Effects of inter-food interval on the variety effect in an instrumental food-seeking task. Clarifying the role of habituation. Appetite, 84, 43-53. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.09.015
  • Vaughan, W. Jr. (1988). Formation of equivalence sets in pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 14, 36-42. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.14.1.36
  • Walk, R. D., & Saltz, E. J. (1965). Discrimination learning with varying numbers of positive and negative stimuli by children of different ages. Psychonomic Science, 2, 95-96. doi: 10.3758/BF03343347
  • Williams, D. I. (1967). Discrimination learning in the pigeon in relation to the number of negative stimuli. Animal Behaviour, 15, 79-81. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(67)80015-0
  • Williams, D. I. (1968). Discrimination learning in the pigeon following unreinforced training on the negative stimulus. Animal Behaviour, 16, 336-337. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(68)90018-3
  • Zentall, T. R., Wasserman, E. A., y Urcuioli, P. J. (2014). Associative concept learning in animals. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 101, 130-151. doi: 10.1002/jeab.55