Cognición social en el síndrome de Williams

  1. Campos García, Ruth
  2. Martínez Castilla, Pastora
  3. Sotillo, María
Revue:
International Journal of Social Psychology, Revista de Psicología Social

ISSN: 0213-4748 1579-3680

Année de publication: 2013

Volumen: 28

Número: 3

Pages: 349-360

Type: Article

DOI: 10.1174/021347413807719139 DIALNET GOOGLE SCHOLAR

D'autres publications dans: International Journal of Social Psychology, Revista de Psicología Social

Objectifs de Développement Durable

Information sur le financement

En un sistema en desarrollo una “foto fija” no ofrece una panorámica del fun-cionamiento (tomado como proceso y como resultado) que puede variar de un momento evolutivo a otro. Así, como hemos visto por ejemplo en relación con el lenguaje, las características fenotípicas que se observan en el SW en la adolescen-cia y la edad adulta no reflejan el estado inicial del sistema. Además, en el caso del SW, un trastorno de origen genético, el punto de partida es diferente de aquel que caracteriza al DT. Como hemos señalado, diferencias iniciales en pro-cesos de bajo nivel (en la planificación de los movimientos sacádicos) en interac-ción con el procesamiento activo que los niños con SW hacen de los inputs ambientales (auditivos) llevan consigo un conjunto de efectos en cascada sobre distintos mecanismos que interaccionan entre sí (atención preferente a los rostros y con ello menor implicación en contextos de interacción triádica, lo que a su vez afecta a los mecanismos de simbolización fundamentales para el desarrollo men-talista y del lenguaje), definiéndose así una trayectoria de desarrollo cualitativa-mente diferente. Como veíamos, el funcionamiento comunicativo y social en SW es un buen ejemplo de cómo determinadas capacidades son el resultado del desarrollo y no del estado inicial.

Financeurs

Références bibliographiques

  • Annaz, D., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Johnson, M. & Thomas, M. (2009). A cross-syndrome study of the development of holistic face recognition in children with autism, Down syndrome, and Williams syndrome. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 456–486. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.11.005
  • Bellugi, U., Lichtenberger, L., Jones, W, Lai, Z. & St. George, M. (2000). The neurocognitive profile of Williams syndrome: A complex pattern of strengths and weakness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 7–29. doi: 10.1162/089892900561959
  • Bellugi, U., Sabo, H. & Vaid, J. (1988). Spatial deficits in children with Williams Syndrome. En J. Stiles-Davis & M. Kritchevsky (Eds.), Spatial cognition: Brain bases and development (pp. 273–298). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Bellugi, U., Wang, P. & Jernigan, T. (1994). Williams Syndrome: an unusual neuropsychological profile. En S. H. Broman & J. Grafman (Eds.), Atypical cognitive deficits in developmental disorders (pp. 23–56). Hillsdale, Nueva York: Erlbaum.
  • Brothers, L. & Ring, B. (1992). A neuroethological framework for the representation of minds. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 4, 107–118. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1992.4.2.107
  • Brown, J., Johnson, M., Paterson, S., Gilmore, R., Gsödl, M., Longhi, E. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2003). Spatial representation and attention in toddlers with Williams syndrome and Down syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1037–1046. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00299-3
  • Campos, R. (2009). Construyendo mentes: Desarrollo de la comprensión de estados mentales en niños con síndrome de Williams y en desarrollo típico. Tesis doctoral no publicada, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid.
  • Campos, R. & Sotillo, M. (2012). Mental state understanding and social interaction. En E. K. Farran & A. Karmiloff-Smith (Eds.), Neurodevelopmenal disorders across the lifespan (pp. 279–295). Nueva York: Oxford University Press.
  • Dai, L., Bellugi, U., Chen, X., Pulst- Korenberg, A., Jarvinen-Pasley, A., Tirosh-Wagner, T., Eis, P., Graham, J., Mills, D., Searcy, Y. & Korenberg, J. (2009). Is it Williams syndrome? GTF2IRD1 implicated in visual-spatial construction and GTF2I in sociability revealed by high resolution arrays. American Journal of Medical Genetics A, 149A, 302–314. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.32652
  • Fidler, D., Hepburn, S., Most, D., Philofsky, A. & Rogers, S. (2007). Emotional responsivity in young children with Williams syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 112, 194–206. doi: 10.1038/ng0993-11
  • Gagliardi, C., Frigerio, E., Burt, D. M., Cazzaniga, I., Perrett, D. I. & Borgatti, R. (2003). Facial expression recognition in Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 41, 733–738. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00178-1
  • Golarai, G., Hong, S., Haas, B., Galaburda, A., Mills, D., Bellugi, U. & Reiss, A. (2010). The fusiform face area is enlarged in Williams syndrome. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 6700–6712. doi: 10.1523/JNEUR0SCI.4268-09.2010
  • Gosch, A., Standing, G. & Pankau, R. (1994). Linguistic abilities in children with Williams-Beuren syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 52, 291–296. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.1320520308
  • Grice, S., Spratling, M., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Halit, H., Csibra, G., De-Haan, M. & Johnson, M. (2001). Disordered visual processing and oscillatory brain activity in autism and Williams syndrome. Neuroreport, 12, 2697–2700. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200108280-00021
  • Haas, B., Hoeft, F., Searcy, Y., Mills, D., Bellugi, U. & Reiss, A. (2010). Individual differences in social behavior predict amygdala response to fearful facial expressions in Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 48, 1283–1288. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.12.030
  • Haas, B., Mills, D., Yam, A., Hoeft, F., Bellugi, U. & Reiss, A. (2009). Genetic influences on sociability: Heightened amygdala reactivity and event-related responses to positive social stimuli in Williams syndrome. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 1132–1139. doi: 10.1523/JNEUR0SCI.5324-08.2009
  • Howlin, P., Davies, M. & Udwin, O. (1998). Cognitive functioning in adults with Williams syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 183–189. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00312
  • John, A., Rowe, M. & Mervis, C. (2009). Referential communication skills of children with Williams syndrome: Understanding when messages are not adequate. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 114, 85–99. doi: 10.1352/2009.114.85-99
  • Johnson, S. & Carey, S. (1998). Knowledge enrichment and conceptual change in folk biology: Evidence from Williams syndrome. Cognitive Psychology, 37, 156–200. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1998.0695
  • Jones, W., Bellugi, U., Lai, Z., Chiles, M., Reilly, J., Lincoln, A. & Adolphs, R. (2000). Hypersociability in Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 30–46. doi: 10.1162/089892900561968
  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1997). Crucial differences between developmental cognitive neuroscience and adult neuropsychology. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13, 513–524. doi: 10.1080/87565649709540693
  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1998). Developmental itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 389–398. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01230-3
  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2007). Atypical epigenesis. Developmental Science, 10, 84–88. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00568.x
  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2012). Brain: The neuroconstructivist approach. En E. K. Farran & A. Karmiloff-Smith, Neurodevelopmenal disorders across the lifespan (pp. 37–58). Nueva York: Oxford University Press.
  • Karmiloff-Smith, A., Broadbent, H., Farran, E., Longhi, E., D'souza, D., Metcalfe, K., Tassabehji, M., Wu, R., Senju, A., Happé, F., Turnpenny, P. & Sansbury, F. (2012). Social cognition in Williams syndrome: Genotype/phenotype insights from partial deletion patients. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 168. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00168
  • Karmiloff-Smith, A., Grant, J., Berthoud, I., Davies, M., Howlin, P. & Udwin, O. (1997). Language and Williams syndrome: How intact is “intact”? Child Development, 68, 246–262. doi: 10.1111/j.14678624.1997.tb01938.x
  • Karmiloff-Smith, A., Klima, E., Bellugi, U., Grant, J. & Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Is there a social module? Language, Face processing and Theory of Mind in individuals with Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 196–208. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1995.7.2.196
  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. & Thomas, M. (2003). What can developmental disorders tell us about the neurocomputational constraints that shape development? The case of Williams syndrome. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 969–990. doi: 10.1017/S0954579403000476
  • Karmiloff-Smith, A., Thomas, M., Annaz, D., Humphreys, K., Ewing, S., Brace, N., Duuren, M., Pike, G., Grice, S. & Campbell, R. (2004). Exploring the Williams syndrome face processing debate: The importance of building developmental trajectories. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 1258–1274. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00322.x
  • Klein-Tasman, B., Mervis, C., Lord, C. & Phillips, K. (2007). Socio-communicative deficits in young children with Williams syndrome: Performance on the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule. Child Neuropsychology, 13, 444–467. doi: 10.1080/09297040601033680
  • Laing, E., Butterworth, G., Ansari, D., Gsödl, M., Longhi, E., Panagiotaki, G., Paterson, S. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2002). Atypical development of language and social communication in toddlers with Williams syndrome. Developmental Science, 5, 233–246. doi: 10.1111/1467-7687.00225
  • Laing, E. & Jarrold, C. (2007). Comprehension of spatial language in Williams syndrome: Evidence for impaired spatial representation of verbal descriptions. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 21, 689–704. doi: 10.1080/02699200701541441
  • Laws, G. & Bishop, D. (2004). Pragmatic language impairment and social deficits in Williams syndrome: A comparison with Down's syndrome and specific language impairment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 39, 45–64. doi: 10.1080/13682820310001615797
  • Levitin, D., Cole, K., Lincoln, A. & Bellugi, U. (2005). Aversion, awareness, and attraction: Investigating claims of hyperacusis in the Williams syndrome phenotype. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 514–523. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00376.x
  • Malenfant, P., Liu X., Hudson M., Qiao Y., Hrynchak, M., Riendeau N., Hildebrand, M., Cohen, I., Chudley, A., Forster-Gibson, C., Mickelson, E., Rajcan-Separovic, E., Lewis, M. & Holden J. (2011). Association of GTF2i in the Williams-Beuren syndrome critical region with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 1459–69. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1389-4
  • Mareschal, D., Johnson, M., Sirois, S., Spratling, M., Thomas, M. & Westermann, G. (2007). Neuroconstructivism, Vol. I: How the brain constructs cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Martens, M., Wilson, S., Dudgeon, P. & Reutens, D. (2009). Approachability and the amygdala: Insights from Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2446–2453. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.04.017
  • Martínez-castilla, P., Sotillo, M. & Campos, R. (2011). Prosodic abilities of Spanish-speaking adolescents and adults with Williams syndrome. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26, 1055–1082. doi: 10.1080/01690965.2010.504058
  • Mervis, C. & Bertrand, J. (1997). Developmental Relations between Cognition and Language: Evidence from Williams Syndrome. En L. B. Adamson & M. A. Romsky (Eds.), Communication and Language Acquisition: Discoveries from Atypical Development (pp. 75–160). Baltimore: Paul Brookes Publishing.
  • Mervis, C., Morris, C., Bertrand, J. & Robinson, F. R. (1999). Williams Syndrome: findings from an integrated program of research. En H. Tager-Flusberg (Ed.), Neurodevelopmental disorders: contribution to a new framework from the cognitive neurosciences (pp. 665–110). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Mervis, C., Morris, C., Klein-Tasman, B., Bertrand, J., Kwitny, S., Appelbaum, L. & Rice, C. (2003). Attentional characteristics of infants and toddlers with Williams syndrome during triadic interactions. Developmental Neuropsychology, 23, 243–68. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2003.9651894
  • Mills, D., Alvarez, T., George, M., Applebaum, L., Bellugi, U. & Neville, H. (2000). Electrophysiological studies of face processing in Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 47–64. doi: 10.1162/089892900561977
  • Paterson, S., Brown, J., Gsödl, M., Johnson, M. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1999). Cognitive modularity and genetic disorders. Science, 286, 2355–2358. doi: 10.1126/science.286.5448.2355
  • Plesa-Skwerer, D., Faja, S., Schofield, C., Verbalis, A. & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2006a). Perceiving facial and vocal expressions of emotion in individuals with Williams syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 111, 15–26. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2006)111[15:PFAVE0]2.0.C0;2)
  • Plesa-Skwerer, D., Verbalis, A., Schofield, C., Faja, S. & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2006b). Social-perceptual abilities in adolescents and adults with Williams syndrome. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23, 338–349. doi: 10.1080/02643290542000076
  • Porter, M., Coltheart, M. & Langdon, R. (2007). The neuropsychological basis of hypersociability in Williams and Down syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2839–2849. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.05.006
  • Porter, M., Coltheart, M. & Langdon, R. (2008). Theory of mind in Williams Syndrome assessed using a nonverbal task. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 806–814. doi: 10.1007/s1083-007-0447-4
  • Reilly, J., Klima, E. & Bellugi, U. (1990). Once more with feeling: Affect and language in atypical populations. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 367–391. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400005782
  • Riby, D., Doherty-Sneddon, G. & Bruce, V. (2008). Atypical unfamiliar face processing in Williams syndrome: What can it tell us about typical familiarity effects? Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 13, 47–58. doi: 10.1080/13546800701779206
  • Riby, D. & Hacock, P. (2009). Do faces capture attention of individuals with Williams syndrome or autism? Evidence from tracking eye movements. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 421–431. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0641-z
  • Rossen, M., Jones, W., Wang, P. & Klima, E. (1995). Face processing: Remarkable sparing in Williams syndrome. Genetic Counseling, 6, 138–140.
  • Sotillo, M., García Nogales, M. A. & Campos, R. (2007). Teoría de la mente y lenguaje: El caso del síndrome de Williams. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 30, 459–74. doi: 10.1174/021037007781787534
  • Stojanovik, V. (2006). Social interaction deficits and conversational inadequacy in Williams syndrome. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 19, 157–173. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2005.11.005
  • Stojanovik, V., Perkins, M. & Howard, S. (2004). Williams syndrome and specific language impairment do not support claims for developmental double dissociations and innate modularity. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 17, 403–424. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2004.01.002
  • Sullivan, K. & Tager-Flusberg, H. (1999). Second-order belief attribution in Williams syndrome: intact or impaired? American Journal on Mental Retardation, 104, 523–532. doi: 10.1352/0895017(1999)104<0523:SBAIWS>2.0.CG;2
  • Tager-Flusberg, H. & Sullivan, K. (2000). A componential view of theory of mind: Evidence from Williams syndrome. Cognition, 16, 59–89. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00069-X
  • Tager-Flusberg, H., Sullivan, K. & Boshart, J. (1997). Executive functions and performance on false belief tasks. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13, 487–493. doi: 10.1080/87565649709540689
  • Thomas, M., Van Duuren, M., Purser, H., Mareschal, D., Ansari, D. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2010). The development of metaphorical language comprehension in typical development and in Williams syndrome. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106, 99–224. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.12.007
  • Volterra, V., Capirci, O., Pezzini, G., Sabbadini, L. & Vicari, S. (1996). Linguistic abilities in Italian children with Williams syndrome. Cortex, 32, 663–677.
  • Young, E., Lipina, T., Tam, E., Mandel, A., Clapcote, S., Bechard, A., Chambers J, Mount, H., Fletcher, P., Roder, J. & Osborne, L. (2008). Reduced fear and aggression and altered serotonin metabolism in Gtf2ird1-targeted mice. Genes Brain and Behavior, 7, 224–234. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2007.00343.x