Razonamiento silogístico e interpretación de premisasun estudio evolutivo

  1. García Madruga, Juan Antonio
Revista:
Journal for the Study of Education and Development, Infancia y Aprendizaje

ISSN: 0210-3702 1578-4126

Año de publicación: 1982

Número: 19-20

Páginas: 175-204

Tipo: Artículo

DOI: 10.1080/02103702.1982.10821951 DIALNET GOOGLE SCHOLAR lock_openDialnet editor

Otras publicaciones en: Journal for the Study of Education and Development, Infancia y Aprendizaje

Objetivos de desarrollo sostenible

Resumen

Errors in syllogistic reasoning have been explained either by erroneous interpretations and combinations of premise meanings, or by response bias made by premises� superficial linguistic features. The present paper postulates that both factors are acting. In this experiment, subjects of four different age levels were tested using Euler�s diagrams verification of premises task followed by a syllogistic reasoning task. Significant improvement of performance with age was found, and hypotheses were confirmed for both tasks. The results are interpreted as a criticism for sequential-linear models that deny interaction between codification and inference phases. Likewise, they support an explanation in terms of two error processes: a general task simplification process and a tendency towards �spontaneous� conclusions made by superficial linguistic codification.

Referencias bibliográficas

  • Agnoli, F. (1978). Content and Structure in Syllogistic Reasoning. Italian Journal of Psychology, Vol. 5, 3, 245–259. [Google Scholar]
  • Bruner, J. S., Goodnow, J. J. y Austin, G. A. (1956). A Study of Thinking, Nueva York, Wiley. Trad, española: El proceso mental en el aprendizaje. Ed. Narcea, Madrid, 1978. [Google Scholar]
  • Bucci, E. (1978). The interpretation of universal affirmative propositions. Cognition, vol. 6(1), 55–77. [Google Scholar]
  • Ceraso, J. y Provitera, A. (1971). Sources of error in Syllogistic reasoning. Cognitive Psychology, 2, 400–410. Trad, española en Delval, J. A. (ed.). Investigaciones sobre lógica y psicología. Alianza Universidad, Madrid, 1977. [Google Scholar]
  • Chapman, L. J. y Chapman, J. P. (1959). Atmosphere effect reexamined. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 220–226. Trad, española en Delval, J. A. (ed.) Investigaciones sobre lógica y Psicología. Alianza Universidad, Madrid 1977. [Google Scholar]
  • Dickstein, L. S. (1975). Effect of instructions and premise order on errors in Syllogistic reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 1, 376–384. [Google Scholar]
  • Dickstein, L. S. (1978 a). The effect of figure in syllogistic reasoning. Memory and Cognition, 6, 76–83. [Crossref], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]
  • Dickstein, L. S. (1978 b). Error processes in syllogistic reasoning. Memory and Cognition, 6, 537–543. [Google Scholar]
  • Eidens, H. (1929). Experimentalle Untersuchungen über den Denkverlauf bei unmittelbaren Folgerungen. Archiv für die Gesamte Psychologie, 71, 1–66. [Google Scholar]
  • Erickson, J. R. (1974). A set analysic theory of behavior in formal syllogistic reasoning tasks. En Solso, R. L. (ed.) Theories of cognitive psychology: The Loyola Symposium. Nueva Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
  • Ericksonj, R. (1978). Research on Syllogistic Reasoning. En R. Revlin y R. Mayer (eds.). Human Reasoning. Washington D.C.: Winston. [Google Scholar]
  • Evans, J. St. B. T. (1977). Lingüistic factors in reasoning. Qualerly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 297–306. [Taylor & Francis Online], [Google Scholar]
  • Gamadruga, J. A. (1982). Un estudio sobre el efecto de la figura en el razonamiento silogístico. Estudios de Psicología (en prensa). [Google Scholar]
  • Inhelder, B. y Piaget, J. (1959). La genèse des structures logiques elementaires. Classifications et sériations. Neuchâtel: Delachaux y Niestlè. Trad, castellana de M. Riani: La génesis de las estructuras lógicas elementales. B. Aires: Guadalupe, 1967 [Google Scholar]
  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1975). Models of deductions. En R. J. Falmagne (ed.): Reasoning: Representation and Process. Nueva Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. y Steedman, M. (1978). The Psychology of Syllogisms. Cognitive Psychology, 10, 64–99. [Crossref], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]
  • Mayor, J. (1979). La verificación de las premisas como base del razonamiento silogístico. Revista de Psicología General y Aplicada, 415–472. [Google Scholar]
  • Mazzocco, A. (1972). Due experimenti sul ragionamiento deduttivo: il problema della conversa. Revista de Psicologia, 66, 47–65. [Google Scholar]
  • Moshman, D. (1980). Representation and process in reasoning about logical relationshpis. The Journal of General Psychology, 103, 251–262. [Taylor & Francis Online], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]
  • Neimarck, E. D. y Chapman, R. H. (1975). Development of the comprehension of logical quantifiers. En: R. J. Falmagne (Ed.): Reasoning: Representation and Process, Nueva Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
  • Piaget, J. (1970). L'évolution intellectuelle entre l'adolescence et l'age adulte. Rapport sur le III Congrès Internationale FONEME, Milán. Trad. cast, en J. A. Delval (ed.): Lecturas de Psicología del niño. Tomo 2, Madrid, Alianza Editorial, 1978. [Google Scholar]
  • Revlin, R.; Ammerman, K.; Peterson, K. y Leirer, V. (1978). Category relations and syllogistic reasoning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 613–625. [Google Scholar]
  • Revlin, R. y Leirer, V. (1978). The effects of personal biases on syllogistic reasoning: Rational decisions from personalized representations. En R. Revlin y R. Mayer (eds.): Human Reasoning. Washington: Winston. [Google Scholar]
  • Revlin, R.; Leirer, V.; Yopp, H. y Yopp, R. (1980). The belief-bias effect in formal reasoning: The influence of knowledge on logic. Memory and Cognition, 8, 584–592. [Crossref], [Google Scholar]
  • Revlis, R. (1975 a). Syllogistic reasoning: Logical decisions from a complex data base. En R. Falmagne (ed.): Reasoning: Representation and Process. Nueva Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
  • Revlis, R. (1975 b). Two models of syllogistic reasoning: Feature selection and conversion. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 180–195. [Crossref], [Google Scholar]
  • Roberge, J. J. (1971). An analysis of response patterns for conditional reasoning schemes. Psychonomic Science, 22, 338–339. [Crossref], [Google Scholar]
  • Rosenthal, D. J. A. (1980). Logical quantifiers in affirmative and negative contexts. The Journal of General Psychology, 102, 99–106. [Taylor & Francis Online], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]
  • Sells, S. B. (1936). The atmosphere effect: an experimental study of reasoning. Archives of Psychology, 29, 3–72. [Google Scholar]
  • Wason, P. C. y Evans, J. St. B. T. (1975). Dual processes in reasoning. Cognition, 3, 141–154. [Crossref], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]
  • Wason, P. C. y Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1972). Psychology of reasoning: structure and content. London, Batsford. Trad. cast, de J. A. Delval: Psicología del Razonamiento, Madrid, ed. Debate, 1980. [Google Scholar]
  • Wildman, T. M. y Fletcher, H. S. (1977). Developmental increases and decreases in solutions of conditional syllogisms problems. Developmental Psychology, 13, 630–636. [Google Scholar]
  • Woodworth, R. S. y Sells, S. B. (1935). An atmosphere effect in formal syllogistic reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 451–460. Trad. cast, en J. A. Delval (ed.): Investigaciones sobre lógica y psicologia, Alianza, Madrid, 1977. [Google Scholar]